The Journal of Nutrition Symposium—Food Addiction: Fact or Fiction? Symposium Overview. Food Addiction: Fact or Fiction?
نویسندگان
چکیده
Food addiction is a pervasive, yet controversial, topic that has gained recent attention in both lay media and the scientific literature. The goal of this series of articles is to use a combination of preclinical and clinical data to determine whether foods, like drugs of abuse, can be addictive, the conditions under which the addiction develops, and the underlying neurophysiological substrates. Operational definitions of addiction that have been used in the treatment of human disorders and to guide research in both humans and animals are presented, and an overview of the symposium articles is provided. We propose that specific foods, especially those that are rich in fat and/or sugar, are capable of promoting ‘‘addiction’’-like behavior and neuronal change under certain conditions. That is, these foods, although highly palatable, are not addictive per se but become so following a restriction/binge pattern of consumption. Such consummatory patterns have been associated with increased risk for comorbid conditions such as obesity, early weight gain, depression, anxiety, and substance abuse as well as with relapse and treatment challenges. The topic of food addiction bears study, therefore, to develop fresh approaches to clinical intervention and to advance our understanding of basic mechanisms involved in loss of control. J. Nutr. 139: 1S–3S, 2009. Food addiction: A pervasive, yet controversial, concept The concept of ‘‘food addiction’’ has gained recent attention in both lay media (;334,000 hits on Google) and in the scientific literature. Food addiction has been implicated in craving, bingeing, and obesity. It has been used to support the avoidance of foods as diverse as carrots (1), cheese, and chocolate as well as foods containing specific compounds such as sugar and casomorphins [see Wadman (2) for an overview]. Treatment of food addiction has even been used as a strategy in the treatment of eating disorders (3). The concept of food addiction, then, is pervasive. Although pervasive, the concept of food addiction is also controversial. Thus, although anecdotal reports abound, few studies have attempted to determine the ‘‘addictive’’ properties of foods using rigorous scientific criteria. One of the problems, of course, is that everyone eats. How can a label of ‘‘addictive’’ be applied to that which supports life itself? Rather than assume that all foods are addictive, we propose that some foods are more addictive than others, especially foods rich in fat and/or sugar. These typically are the foods that become forbidden, are those on which people binge, and are often energy dense. Even so, we also propose that these foods, although highly palatable, are not addictive, per se, but become so following a restriction/ binge pattern of consumption. How to define addiction in humans To define addiction, we turn to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR), in which criteria for substance dependence are defined. Sweet and fatty foods are not considered ‘‘substances’’ in the classical sense. Substances are typically interpreted as drugs of abuse such as cocaine, ethanol, heroin, etc. For a person to be considered dependent on (or addicted to) any given substance, at least 3 of the following 7 criteria must be met at any time within a given year: 1) tolerance (more drug is needed for the same effect), 2) withdrawal, 3) taking a larger amount of the substance or taking the substance for a longer period than was intended, 4) experiencing a persistent desire for the substance or an inability to reduce or control its use, 5) spending much time seeking or consuming the substance or recovering from its effects, 6) use of the substance interfering with important activities, and 7) use of the substance continuing despite known adverse consequences. If either criterion 1 or 2 is met, then physiological dependence is diagnosed. However, a diagnosis of substance dependence does not require that criterion 1 or 2 be met. Thus, human substance dependence (or addiction) can be diagnosed using entirely behavioral criteria (4). 1 Published as a supplement to The Journal of Nutrition. Presented as part of the symposium ‘‘Food Addiction: Fact or Fiction’’ given at the 2008 Experimental Biology meeting, April 8, 2008 in San Diego, CA. The symposium was sponsored by the American Society for Nutrition and supported by an educational grant from The National Institute on Drug Abuse, The National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, and The National Dairy Council. The symposium was chaired by Rebecca L. Corwin and Patricia S. Grigson. 2 Author disclosures: R. Corwin and P. Grigson, no conflicts of interest. 3 Supported by MH67943 (R.L.C.) and DA09815 (P.S.G.). The symposium was dedicated to Dr. Ann Kelley, a stellar scientist, colleague, and friend. * To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: [email protected]. 0022-3166/08 $8.00 a 2009 American Society for Nutrition. doi:10.3945/jn.108.097691. 1S The Journal of Nutrition. First published ahead of print January 28, 2009 as doi: 10.3945/jn.108.097691. Copyright (C) 2009 by the American Society for Nutrition by gest on O cber 0, 2017 jn.nition.org D ow nladed fom Is there evidence for food addiction in humans? A key feature of any addiction is loss of control. The elemental behavior in a food addiction is the consumption of food; loss of control is manifest by either more frequent and/or larger meals. A meal that is larger than would normally be consumed in a given period of time under similar circumstances is a binge. Although not labeled as such, several of the diagnostic/research criteria for the bingeing-related eating disorders approximate criteria for substance dependence (4). These include the following: 1) binge-type consumption (taking the substance in larger amounts than was intended); 2) loss of control over eating (inability to control its use); 3) bingeing occurs at least twice a week for 3 mo in bulimia nervosa or for 6 mo in binge eating disorder (taking the substance for a longer period of time than was intended); 4) for bulimia nervosa, bingeing is followed by inappropriate compensatory behavior such as vomiting, laxative use, enemas, etc.; for binge eating disorder, bingeing is accompanied by disgust, embarrassment, guilt, and marked distress (use continues despite adverse consequences). A diagnosis of bulimia nervosa or binge eating disorder, then, could be considered to fulfill at least 3 of the criteria for substance dependence. In addition, although craving is not considered one of the diagnostic criteria for the bingeing-related eating disorders, craving has been associated with binge eating (5), and food craving activates areas of the brain reported to be involved in drug craving (6). Thus, both behavioral and neurobiological evidence support the conclusion that food, under conditions discussed more fully by Pelchat (6), can induce an addiction-like state in human subjects. Finally, it is important to note that comorbidities associated with binge behavior, such as obesity, early weight gain, depression, anxiety, and substance abuse, as described by Avena et al. (7), make the potential negative impact of this behavior on individuals and on society substantial. Food addiction: Animal studies and underlying substrates Although human studies are critical to our understanding of the addiction process, animal studies are crucial to our understanding of basic mechanisms that underlie that process. Work such as that done by Ann Kelley, to whom this symposium was dedicated, and her colleagues has clearly shown how homeostatic mechanisms involved in feeding interface with those involved in reward and addiction (8). Two of the articles from this symposium describe work in which rat models were used to examine basic mechanisms common to the consumption of food and drugs. Lutter and Nestler (9) describe how withdrawal from a high-fat diet leads to neurochemical responses comparable to those induced by withdrawal from drug (9). They also describe how affective states such as depression, a key feature of drug withdrawal, can modify endogenous signals involved in food intake regulation. Avena et al. (7) provide convincing evidence that bingeing on sugar can induce behavioral and neuronal changes in rats similar to those induced by drugs of abuse. In addition, Pelchat (6) discusses how external stimuli, such as cues, can modulate food seeking and food intake in important ways via classical conditioning. All of these articles illustrate the fact that food, at least in part, acts on the same neuronal systems as do drugs of abuse. How to define addiction in animals As with the study of human behavior, one challenge in the study of animal behavior is to determine when the transition from use to addiction has occurred. Because addiction in humans is primarily defined behaviorally, any claim of ‘‘addiction’’ in animals would be well served by the demonstration of clear behavioral differences between addicted and nonaddicted subjects. Over the past several years, operational criteria have been developed for rats self-administering cocaine that parallel the DSM-IV-TR criteria (10–13). In addition to tolerance and withdrawal, the following behavioral criteria have been proposed: 1) escalation of intake across time (taking a large amount; loss of control); 2) willingness to work harder to get the substance (high motivation to obtain and consume the substance); 3) greater responding during periods when the drug is not available (increased time spent seeking the drug); and 4) increased responding despite aversive consequences (use continues despite adverse consequences). Is there evidence for food addiction in animals? Although several reports have used the proposed rat behavioral criteria when studying addiction to drugs, few have systematically applied similar criteria when studying addiction to food. Work from our group has made use of some of these criteria to demonstrate that addiction-like behavior (escalation of intake, increased willingness to work for fat or cocaine, increased cocaine seeking) can be induced in rats with a history of bingeing on fat (14–16). Moreover, we found that use of a more restricted access schedule to fat led to more addiction-like behavior in rats. The article by Avena et al. (7) describes strong evidence for addiction-like behavioral and neurological alterations in rats bingeing on sugar and how some of these effects differ from those that occur when rats binge on fat. Summary and conclusion Taken together, the articles from this symposium provide evidence that neurological similarities exist in the response of humans (6) and rats (7,9) to foods and to drugs. Two of the reports (6,7), as well as our own work (14–16), suggest that even highly palatable food is not addictive in and of itself. Rather, it is the manner in which the food is presented (i.e., intermittently) and consumed (i.e., repeated, intermittent ‘‘gorging’’) that appears to entrain the addiction-like process. Such consummatory patterns are associated with increased risk for comorbid complications as well as relapse and make treatment particularly challenging. The topic of food addiction bears study, therefore, to develop fresh approaches to clinical intervention and to advance our understanding of basic mechanisms involved in loss of control. Other articles in this supplement include references (6,7,9).
منابع مشابه
Using Iron-Chelating Agents in Critically Ill Patients with Iron Overload. Fact or Fiction?
Recently, some evidence has shown that the failure of iron homeostasis may occur in critically ill patients and can lead to iron overload. Elevated ferritin levels as a body iron burden index in critically ill patients may be associated with depressed level of consciousness and greater mortality. However, the necessity of using iron-chelating agents in clinical situation is still unknown for th...
متن کاملThe 17th congress of parenteral and enteral Nutrition society of Asia (PENSA) and the first symposium on fasting and health, 6th-8th December 2016, Tehran, Iran
The 17th Congress of Parenteral and Enteral Nutrition Society of Asia (PENSA 2016) and the first Symposium on Fasting and Health will be held on 6th to 8th December 2016 at Olympic Hotel, Tehran, Iran. The congress is hosted by Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Semnan University of Medical Sciences and Scientific Committee of Iranian Nutrition Society. This congress is organized with the p...
متن کاملThe System of Engagement in a Sample of Prose Fiction and the News
Emerging within Systemic Linguistics, Appraisal/Evaluation is a framework for analyzing the language of evaluation, providing techniques for the systematic analysis of evaluation and stance as they operate in whole texts and in groupings of texts. There are three systems in the Appraisal framework: Attitude, Engagement, and Graduation. This study sets out to analyze the use of the system of Eng...
متن کاملBack by Popular Demand: A Narrative Review on the History of Food Addiction Research
In recent years, the concept of food addiction has gained more and more popularity. This approach acknowledges the apparent parallels between substance use disorders and overeating of highly palatable, high-caloric foods. Part of this discussion includes that "hyperpalatable" foods may have an addictive potential because of increased potency due to certain nutrients or additives. Although this ...
متن کاملThe Power of Fiction: How Literature Educated and Reformed American Society
This paper focuses on a number of American stories which helped educate people and bring about legal or social change. There are many stories which caused major or minor legal and political change, particularly, in the United States. Some of them are written by Herman Melville, Harriet Beecher Stowe, Upton Sinclair and Sidney Kingsley. After the publication of White Jacket by Melville the novel...
متن کامل